The effect of choice of exercise mode on psychological responses

https://doi.org/10.1016/S1469-0292(02)00053-5Get rights and content

Abstract

Objectives: Choice has been linked to both motivation and psychological responses to exercise. This paper investigates participants’ psychological responses when they completed an exercise session where there was a high-preference exercise mode as opposed to a low-preference exercise mode. High- and low-preference were established by participants indicating their preferred choice of exercise from three standard modes used for aerobic work.

Method: Participants were 20 low-active adults who exercised for 20 min on each mode of exercise (high-preference versus low-preference). Affect, ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) and heart rate (HR) were recorded every 5 min during exercise, and affect and HR were assessed pre-exercise and 5 min post-exercise.

Results: Repeated measures analysis of variance revealed condition and time main effects for affect sub-scales and RPE and a time main effect for HR. Affect sub-scales, RPE and HR generally increased over time. RPE, Fatigue and Psychological Distress were higher in the low-preference condition and positive well-being was higher in the high-preference condition.

Conclusions: Affect and RPE were influenced by the preferred choice manipulation. Affect was more positive, and RPE lower, in the high-preference versus low-preference condition while work rate remained constant across conditions.

Introduction

More than 20 reviews have been published in the last decade concerned with the relationship between physical activity and psychological responses. Research generally supports that physical activity can have an acute, as well as a chronic, effect on these responses (e.g. Landers and Arent, 2001, Petruzzello, Landers, Hatfield, Kubitz and Salazar, 1991). However, variables such as exercise intensity, fitness of participants, past experience, environment, goal orientation and preference may all influence these responses. For example, high intensity exercise can be associated with decrements in affective state post-exercise especially in low fit or sedentary individuals (Steptoe and Bolton, 1988, Tuson, Sinyor and Pelletier, 1995) while moderate exercise intensity has not been associated with decrements in affective state regardless of the activity level of individuals (Parfitt, Markland and Holmes, 1994, Steptoe and Bolton, 1988, Van Landuyt, Ekkekakis, Hall and Petruzzello, 2000). Environments perceived to foster a task climate have been associated with less negative affect and more positive affect compared to an ego climate (Ntoumanis & Biddle, 1999a), while task goal orientations have been shown to produce more positive affective responses (Ntoumanis & Biddle, 1999b).

A number of authors have recognised that perference may be an important moderator in the relationship between exercise intensity and psychological response (Ekkekakis and Petruzzello, 1999, Morgan, 1997, Rudolph and Kim, 1996), although few studies have experimentally explored such a relationship. Previous research that has considered the effects of preferred exercise intensity on psychological affect (Dishman, Farquhar and Cureton, 1994, Eston, Parfitt and Tucker, 1998, Parfitt, Rose and Markland, 2000) generally supports a more positive outcome following the preferred intensity condition in individuals who are active. For example, state anxiety decreased in high-active participants compared to low-active participants (Dishman et al., 1994), affect assessed using the Feeling Scale (Rejeski, Best, Griffith, & Kenney, 1987) improved post-exercise in both high- and low-active participants (Eston et al., 1998), and even when participants chose to work physiologically harder, their affect remained high and positive and ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) were low and stable (Parfitt et al., 2000). One explanation for these positive outcomes can be discussed in relation to Deci and Ryan’s (1985) theory of self-determination. Choice over one’s actions will encourage an internal locus of causality which should enhance intrinsic motivation for the behaviour (Deci & Ryan, 1985). Both Vallerand (1997) and Deci and Ryan (1985) suggest that the most positive affective, cognitive and behavioural consequences result from being intrinsically motivated or having more self-determined forms of behavioural regulation.

An alternative explanation for such positive outcomes could be associated with flow theory (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). In the process of making a choice, participants are likely to match their personal competencies with the challenge of the task and as such maximise the potential for positive outcomes. Given these two explanations, it is surprising that empirical studies have not investigated the impact of choice of exercise mode upon psychological responses. Indeed Biddle (2000, p. 82) indicated that “people will ‘feel good’ after exercise they prefer, and feel ‘less good’ after exercise that is not to their liking”.

From a health promotion perspective, if we are able to identify factors within the exercise environment that foster more positive affect, we may be able to improve exercise participation. The purpose of this study, therefore, was to compare psychological states after 20 min on a ‘high-preference’ mode of exercise versus 20 min on a ‘low-preference’ mode of exercise. It was hypothesised that participants would report more positive psychological states during and following exercise in the high-preference mode than the low-preference mode.

Section snippets

Method

Twenty (10 male, 10 female) self-reported low-active participants (active less than twice a week) volunteered to take part in the study (mean age=20.55, SD=1.46years). Approximately 50% of the participants were university students; the rest lived and worked locally to the university. Participants attended the laboratory on three occasions, 3 days apart. They signed an informed consent form before commencing the familiarisation session. During this session, participants experienced the three

Results

In the experimental conditions, 60% of the participants chose to exercise on the cycle ergometer, 35% on the treadmill and 5% on the rower (high-preference), while 15% exercised on the cycle, 25% on the treadmill and 60% on the rower in the low-preference condition. Repeated measures (condition× time×gender) analysis of variance were conducted on the data. To reduce the risk of type I error, alpha was set at 1%. Analyses revealed no significant interactions or gender effects, but significant

Discussion

The results partially support the hypothesis, with PWB higher and RPE, PD and Fatigue lower in the high-preference condition than in the low-preference condition. These results suggest that preference for mode of exercise can influence psychological responses both during and after exercise. It may be that the preference manipulation, which recognised participants’ choice in the high-preference condition, may have influenced feelings of self-determination. Certainly, previous literature has

References (29)

  • R.J. Vallerand

    Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation

    Advances in Experimental Social Psychology

    (1997)
  • R.R. Yeung

    The acute effects of exercise on mood state

    Journal of Psychosomatic Research

    (1996)
  • S.J.H. Biddle

    Emotion, mood and physical activity

  • G.A.V. Borg

    Perceived exertion as an indicator of somatic stress

    Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine

    (1970)
  • C.B. Cooke

    Maximal oxygen uptake, economy and efficiency

  • M. Csikszentmihalyi

    Flow: The psychology of optimal experience

    (1990)
  • E.L. Deci et al.

    Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior

    (1985)
  • R.K. Dishman et al.

    Responses to preferred intensities of exercise in men differing in activity levels

    Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise

    (1994)
  • P. Ekkekakis et al.

    Acute aerobic exercise and affect: current status, problems and prospects regarding dose-responses

    Sports Medicine

    (1999)
  • R.G. Eston et al.

    Ratings of perceived exertion and psychological affect during preferred work rate in high- and low-active men [abstract]

    Journal of Sports Sciences

    (1998)
  • R.G. Eston et al.

    Reliability of ratings of perceived effort for the regulation of exercise intensity

    British Journal of Sports Medicine

    (1988)
  • L. Gauvin et al.

    The Exercise-Induced Feeling Inventory: development and initial validation

    Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology

    (1993)
  • D.M. Landers et al.

    Physical activity and mental health

  • C.L. Lox et al.

    The Subjective Exercise Experience Scale (SEES): factorial validity and effects of acute exercise

    Journal of Social Behaviour and Personality

    (1994)
  • Cited by (57)

    • Positive affect moderates inhibitory control and positive affect following a single bout of self-select aerobic exercise

      2022, Psychology of Sport and Exercise
      Citation Excerpt :

      Determining the ideal exercise parameters for improvements in cognition and affect remains complicated. Most of the cognitive literature utilizes an experimenter-controlled prescription-based approach with a fixed intensity (Chang et al., 2012; Lambourne & Tomporowski, 2010; Verburgh et al., 2014), while a majority of the affect literature utilizes a participant-controlled preference-based approach (Daley & Maynard, 2003; Miller et al., 2005; Parfitt & Gledhill, 2004). The preference-based methodology is driven by the self-determination theory which suggests that increasing autonomy in the selection of exercise behavior increases perceptions of competence and self-efficacy (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Teixeira et al., 2012) resulting in increased likelihood to engage in similar future behaviors.

    • Acute aerobic exercise increases respiratory sinus arrythmia reactivity and recovery to a sad film among individuals at risk for depression

      2020, International Journal of Psychophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Third, we used cycle ergometry for the acute exercise in this study and for the rest condition, participants were asked to sit on the ergometer to equate the two experimental conditions. Other modes of exercise (e.g., running, swimming), and individual exercise preferences may influence post-exercise cardiovascular and affective responses (Miller et al., 2005; Parfitt and Gledhill, 2004). Further, asking participants to sit on a cycle ergometer may evoke demand characteristics that influence RSA and affective responses.

    • Choice and perceptions of exercise: A test of three moderating variables

      2018, Psychology of Sport and Exercise
      Citation Excerpt :

      When BMIS scores were submitted to the 3 × 2 between-subjects ANCOVA, the analysis yielded no significant effects on this assessment of post-exercise mood (ps > .10). Previous research has found that the opportunity for choice between different types of exercise activities and intensity levels can improve exercise enjoyment and intentions (e.g., Parfitt & Gledhill, 2004). In the present study, we tested whether choice over ostensibly different exercise programs in the same domain (i.e., on an exercise bicycle) alters the positivity of an exercise experience, attitudes towards physical activity, post-exercise mood, intentions to engage in future exercise, and desire to receive more information about exercise.

    • Disentangling the effects of choice and intensity on affective response to and preference for self-selected- versus imposed-intensity physical activity

      2013, Psychology of Sport and Exercise
      Citation Excerpt :

      It remains unclear, however, to what extent this deterioration in affective valence is the result of (a) lack of choice when people are pushed to a higher intensity, versus (b) engaging in PA of a higher intensity (Williams, 2008; see Fig. 1). According to self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985), when PA intensity is freely chosen—versus imposed—it should result in greater perceived choice, and thus a more positive affective response (Daley & Maynard, 2003; Parfitt & Gledhill, 2004). Theoretically, the link between perceived choice and affective response should be independent of differences in PA intensities.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text