Skip to main content

Functional neural correlates of reduced physiological falls risk

Abstract

Background

It is currently unclear whether the function of brain regions associated with executive cognitive processing are independently associated with reduced physiological falls risk. If these are related, it would suggest that the development of interventions targeted at improving executive neurocognitive function would be an effective new approach for reducing physiological falls risk in seniors.

Methods

We performed a secondary analysis of 73 community-dwelling senior women aged 65 to 75 years old who participated in a 12-month randomized controlled trial of resistance training. Functional MRI data were acquired while participants performed a modified Eriksen Flanker Task - a task of selective attention and conflict resolution. Brain volumes were obtained using MRI. Falls risk was assessed using the Physiological Profile Assessment (PPA).

Results

After accounting for baseline age, experimental group, baseline PPA score, and total baseline white matter brain volume, baseline activation in the left frontal orbital cortex extending towards the insula was negatively associated with reduced physiological falls risk over the 12-month period. In contrast, baseline activation in the paracingulate gyrus extending towards the anterior cingulate gyrus was positively associated with reduced physiological falls risk.

Conclusions

Baseline activation levels of brain regions underlying response inhibition and selective attention were independently associated with reduced physiological falls risk. This suggests that falls prevention strategies may be facilitated by incorporating intervention components - such as aerobic exercise - that are specifically designed to induce neurocognitive plasticity.

Trial Registration

ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT00426881

Introduction

Falls are a major health care problem for seniors and health care systems. They are the third leading cause of chronic disability worldwide [1] and approximately 30% of community-dwellers over the age of 65 years experience one or more falls every year [2]. Importantly, 5% of falls result in fracture, with one-third of those being hip fractures.

Key risk factors for falls include reduced physiological function, such as impaired balance, [3, 4] and cognitive impairment [2]. Recent evidence suggests that even mild reductions in cognitive abilities among otherwise healthy community-dwelling older adults increase physiological falls risk [5–8]. Specifically, evidence suggests that reduced executive functions -- the ability to concentrate, to attend selectively, and to plan and strategize -- are associated with increased falls risk among seniors without cognitive impairment and dementia [5, 6, 9–11].

Currently, the neural basis for the association between reduced executive functions and falls is unclear. Evidence from neuroimaging studies provides insight to possible underlying mechanisms. Specifically, cerebral white matter lesions (or leukoaraiosis) are associated with both reduced executive functions [12] and gait and balance abnormalities [13–16]. Cerebral white matter lesions may interrupt frontal lobe circuits responsible for normal gait and balance or they may interfere with long loop reflexes mediated by deep white matter sensory and motor tracts [15]. In addition, the periventricular and subcortical distribution of white matter lesions could interrupt the descending motor fibers arising from medial cortical areas, which are important for lower extremity motor control [16]. However, while the results of these neuroimaging studies contribute to our appreciation of the importance of brain structure to physiological falls risk, they do not provide specific guidance for refining or developing falls prevention strategies because white matter lesions are not currently modifiable once they present. Studies have also demonstrated the contribution of brain volume to physiological falls risk. Specifically, reduced grey matter volume within sensorimotor and frontal parietal regions of the brain is associated with both reduced gait speed and impaired balance [17, 18].

Of particular relevance to falls prevention, targeted exercise training is beneficial for both brain volume, as assessed by MRI, and brain function, as assessed by fMRI [19]. What has not been well examined to date is the contribution of brain function to physiological falls risk. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we previously demonstrated that reduced activity in the posterior lobe of the right cerebellum during an executive-challenging cognitive task may be an underlying neural mechanism for increased falls risk [20].

To our knowledge, it is currently unknown whether the function of brain regions responsible for executive functions are independently associated with reduced physiological falls risk after accounting for relevant factors such as baseline age, baseline physiological falls risk, and baseline brain volume. Yet, such knowledge would facilitate the development and refinement of targeted interventions to reduce physiological falls risk in older adults. Thus, we used fMRI to examine the functional neural correlates of executive functioning that are independently associated with reduced physiological falls risk among community-dwelling senior women.

Methods

Participants

The sample for this analysis consisted of a subset of 155 women who consented and completed a 12-month randomized controlled trial of exercise (NCT00426881) that primarily aimed to examine the effect of once-weekly or twice-weekly resistance training compared with a twice-weekly balance and tone exercise intervention on cognitive performance of executive functions. The design and the primary results of the study have been reported elsewhere [21].

We recruited and randomized 155 senior women who: 1) were aged 65-75 years; 2) were living independently in their own home; 3) obtained a score ≥ 24 on the Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE) [22]; and 4) had a visual acuity of at least 20/40, with or without corrective lenses. We excluded those who: 1) had a diagnosed neurodegenerative disease (e.g., AD) and/or stroke; 2) were taking psychotropic drugs; 3) did not speak and understand English; 4) had moderate to significant impairment with ADLs as determined by interview; 5) were taking cholinesterase inhibitors within the last 12 months; 6) were taking anti-depressants within the last six months; or 7) were on oestrogen replacement therapy within the last 12 months.

Ethical approval was obtained from the Vancouver Coastal Health Research Institute (V06-0326) and the University of British Columbia's Clinical Research Ethics Board (H06-0326). All participants provided written informed consent.

Randomization

The randomization sequence was generated by http://www.randomization.com and was concealed until interventions were assigned. This sequence was held independently and remotely by the Research Coordinator. Participants were enrolled and randomised by the Research Coordinator to one of three groups: once-weekly resistance training (1x RT), twice-weekly resistance training (2x RT), or twice-weekly balance and tone (BAT).

Exercise Intervention

Resistance Training

All classes were 60 minutes in duration. The protocol for this program was progressive and high-intensity in nature. Both a Keiser® Pressurized Air system and free weights were used to provide the training stimulus. Other key strength exercises included mini-squats, mini-lunges, and lunge walks.

Balance and Tone

This program consisted of stretching exercises, range of motion exercises, kegals, balance exercises, and relaxation techniques. This group served to control for confounding variables such as physical training received by traveling to the training centres, social interaction, and lifestyle changes secondary to study participation.

Descriptive Variables

Global cognition was assessed using the MMSE [22]. We used the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) [23] to screen for depression. Functional Comorbidity Index was calculated to estimate the degree of comorbidity associated with physical functioning [24]. This scale's score is the total number of comorbidities.

Dependent Variable: Physiological Falls Risk

Physiological falls risk was assessed using the short form of the physiological profile assessment (PPA; Prince of Wales Medical Research Institute, AUS) to assess physiological falls risk. The PPA measures five domains of physiological functioning - dominant hand reaction time, postural sway, contrast sensitivity, proprioception, and dominant quadriceps strength - and computes a global falls risk score that has 75% accuracy for predicting falls. Global PPA scores < 0 indicate low falls risk, 0 to 1 indicate mild falls risk, 1 to 2 indicate moderate falls risk, and scores > 2 indicate high falls risk. We calculated change in physiological falls risk as the difference score between the baseline global PPA score and the trial completion PPA score; higher PPA change scores indicate greater reductions in physiological falls risk.

Independent Variables of Interest

Brain Structure: Anatomical MRI

Baseline brain volume was measured via high-resolution, T1-weighted structural MRI images obtained using a Philips Achieva 3T scanner (TR = 8 ms, TE = 3.7 ms, bandwidth = 2.26 kHz, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm). Brain tissue volume, normalized for subject head size, was estimated with SIENAX [25], part of FSL (FMRIB's Software Library, Version 4.1.4) [26]. SIENAX starts by extracting brain and skull images from the single whole-head T1 image [27]. The brain image was then affine-registered to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) 152 space [28, 29]. Next, tissue-type segmentation with partial volume estimation was carried out [30] in order to calculate baseline total volume of brain tissue, total white matter volume, and total grey matter volume.

Brain Function: Functional MRI

Transverse echo-planar imaging (EPI) images in-plane with the AC-PC line were acquired using a gradient-echo pulse sequence and sequential slice acquisition (TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, 36 contiguous slices at 3 mm skip 1 mm, in-plane resolution of 128 × 128 pixels reconstructed in a FOV of 240 mm). Each functional run began with four TR's during which no data were acquired to allow for steady-state tissue magnetization. A total of 148 EPI volumes were collected in each functional run, and a total of 6 functional runs were collected for each participant.

During scanning, participants performed a modified Eriksen flanker task [31] -- a task that engages the executive cognitive processes of selective attention and conflict resolution (Figure 1). Participants viewed displays with an arrow at central fixation, flanked by a pair of arrows on either side. In half the trials, the flanking arrows pointed in the same direction as the central arrow cue (e.g., < < < < <; congruent condition), and in the other half, the flanking arrows pointed in the opposite direction (e.g., > > < > >; incongruent condition). There were four event types based on whether the central arrow was congruent versus incongruent with the distracter arrows and whether it pointed to the left or right. A central fixation cross was presented for 500 milliseconds at the beginning of each trial. The target stimulus (arrows) was then shown for 2000 milliseconds. An average of 13500 milliseconds of blank screen was presented between each trial, jittered between 11500 and 15000 milliseconds. Each participant underwent six successive five-minute blocks where they were presented with 17 trials that were first-order counterbalanced such that congruent and incongruent trials followed each other equally. The participants' task on each trial was to signal the direction the central arrow points via a simple key press. Reaction time was recorded in milliseconds. At the end of the sessions, a high-resolution scan allowed each participant's anatomical and functional images to be co-registered during data analysis.

Figure 1
figure 1

The Flanker Task. Participants were presented with a 13.5-sec fixation cross, which was followed by a 500 milliseconds pre-cue that informed participants that the critical stimulus will appear soon. Finally, an array of five arrows was on the screen. Participants responded to the orientation of the central arrow cue by pressing a button with their left hand if the central arrow cue pointed to the left and with their right hand if the central arrow cue pointed to the right. During one half of the trials, the flanking arrows faced in the same direction as the central arrow cue (i.e., congruent trials), and during the other half, they pointed in the opposite direction as the central arrow cue (i.e., incongruent trials). These stimuli remained on the screen for 2,000 milliseconds. Each participant underwent six successive five-minute blocks where they were presented with 17 trials that are first-order counterbalanced such that consistent and inconsistent trials followed each other equally [31]. This paradigm is sensitive to age-related decrements in attention control [48].

Functional MRI data were processed and analyzed using SPM2 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). For each participant, the EPI images were corrected for motion using the INRIalign toolbox for SPM2 (http://www-sop.inria.fr/epidaure/software/INRIAlign/). The resulting images were spatially-normalized into MNI stereotaxic coordinates using the EPI template provided with SPM2 [32], and spatially smoothed using an isotropic 8 mm Gaussian kernel. For each participant, the smoothed, normalized EPI data were analyzed via multiple regression using a fixed-effects general linear model [33]. In particular, the event-related responses to the onsets of the stimuli was examined, with each participant's model including four event-related regressors: 1) one for each combination of target type (i.e., left or right); 2) and distracter condition (i.e., congruent or incongruent). Regressors were based on the canonical event-related hemodynamic response function, temporal derivatives of the event-related responses were included as additional regressors, and low-frequency scanner and/or physiological noise was modeled via linear, quadratic, and cubic regressors of non-interest. Group-level analyses were then based on a random-effects model using one-sample t-tests, with a threshold of p < 0.05, corrected, and a minimum extent threshold of 10 contiguous voxels. Mean beta values reported for clusters identified in the group-level data were extracted from the SPM2 data files using custom scripts implemented in MATLAB (The MATHWORKS Inc., Natick, MA). The group-level cluster means were calculated by first determining each participant's mean beta across all voxels in the given cluster. All reported voxel coordinates were converted to Talairaich coordinates [34] using the mni2tal MATLAB script (http://imaging.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/imaging/MniTalairach). The mean beta values were then imported to SPSS.

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive data are reported for variables of interest. Data were analyzed using SPSS Windows Version 18.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). The associations between the variables were determined using the Pearson product moment coefficient of correlation.

A multiple linear regression model was constructed to determine the independent association of the neural correlates of executive functioning, as assessed by fMRI, with change in physiological falls risk over the 12-month intervention study, as assessed by PPA. Baseline age, experimental group, and baseline physiological falls risk were statistically controlled by entering these three variables into the regression model first. These independent variables were determined from the results of the Pearson product moment coefficient of correlation analyses (i.e., baseline PPA score) and based on biological relevance, such as experimental group and age.

Baseline total brain volume, total white matter volume, and total grey matter volume were then entered into regression model and only those that significantly improved the model were included (i.e., stepwise). Finally, regions of the brain (i.e., clusters) showing increases in the hemodynamic response on incongruent relative to congruent trials of the flanker task were then entered into the model using a stepwise approach. Alpha was set at p ≤ 0.05.

Results

Participants and Variables of Interest

Of the 155 participants who consented and were randomized at baseline, 135 completed the 12-month trial. Seventy-three of the 135 participants consented and completed baseline MRI and fMRI scanning.

Table 1 reports the baseline descriptive statistics for this cohort. The mean baseline PPA score was 0.10, indicating mild falls risk. At the end of the 12-month trial, the 73 women demonstrated a mean change of 0.10 in the PPA score. A paired t-test indicated that this was not a statistically significant change (p = 0.06).

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for variables of interest (N = 73).

Behavioural performance on the flanker task was calculated as percent increase in reaction time to incongruent stimuli, over and above the average reaction time to congruent stimuli {[(incongruent reaction time - congruent reaction time)/congruent reaction time] × 100} [31]. The percent increase measure is derived to reflect interference unbiased by differences in base reaction time. Only correct responses were included in the analysis. Mean interference score for BAT, RT1, and RT2 were 16.59 (SD = 13.07), 19.92 (SD = 2.52), and 27.98 (SD = 13.77), respectively.

Consistent with previous studies using the flanker task, regions showing increases in the hemodynamic response on incongruent relative to congruent trials included bilateral inferior and middle frontal gyri, frontal orbital cortex, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), bilateral precuneus, and the right cerebellum (Figure 2); 14 clusters were identified (Table 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Brain Regions Demonstrating an Increased Hemodynamic Response on Incongruent Relative to Congruent Trials. Data are group-averaged across all 83 participants and shown on a rendered brain provided with SPM2. Data were thresholded at P< 0.05 (corrected) and a minimum cluster size of 10 contiguous voxels. The left OFC/In and right PCG/ACC both contributed significantly to our model predicting change in physiological falls risk.

Table 2 Voxel Cluster Statistics from fMRI.

Correlation Coefficients

Table 3 reports the bivariate correlation coefficients of those variables included in the final multiple linear regression model. Baseline physiological falls risk was positively and significantly associated with change in physiological falls risk (p < 0.001). Baseline total brain volume, total white matter volume, and activation (i.e., hemodynamic response) in the left frontal orbital cortex extending towards the insula (OFC/In) were negatively and significantly associated with change in physiological falls risk (p < 0.05). In our bivariate analysis, age, experimental group, and activation in the right paracingulate gyrus extending towards the anterior cingulate cortex (PCG/ACC) were not associated with change in physiological falls risk (p > 0.26).

Table 3 Multiple linear regression model summary for improved physiological falls risk.

Linear Regression Model

Baseline age, experimental group, and baseline physiological falls risk, accounted for 31.9% of the variance in change in physiological falls risk (Table 3). Adding baseline total white matter volume resulted in an R-square change of 6.4% and significantly improved the regression model (F Change = 7.1, p = 0.01). Adding activation in the left OFC/In to the model resulted in an R-square change of 10.4% and significantly improved the model (F Change = 13.6, p < 0.001). Finally, the inclusion of activation in the right PCG/ACC resulted in significant R-square change of 4.4% (F Change = 6.6, p = 0.02). The total variance accounted by the final model was 53.1% (Table 3). Based on the standardized betas, the left OFC/In was most associated with reduced physiological falls risk.

Discussion

Recent evidence strongly suggests that changes in brain structure with age contribute to problems with mobility [35–39]. However, less is known about the role of brain function [20]. To our knowledge, our study is the first to demonstrate the independent contribution of brain function to reduced physiological falls risk among community-dwelling seniors. Specifically, after accounting for baseline age, experimental group, baseline physiological falls risk, and baseline total white matter volume, activation in the left OFC/In was negatively and independently associated with reduced physiological falls risk in community-dwelling senior women over a 12-month period. In contrast, activation in the PCG/ACC was positively and independently associated with reduced physiological falls risk.

The two regions included in our multiple linear regression model -- the left OFC/In and the right PCG/ACC -- are both part of the neural network associated with response inhibition and selective attention [40–43]. Response inhibition - the ability to avoid unwanted, inappropriate responses - is associated with falls in seniors. For example, Anstey and colleagues [44] reported that senior fallers (both single and recurrent) performed significantly worse on a measure of response inhibition compared to non-fallers. The authors suggested that reduced inhibition results from age-related declines in functioning of the prefrontal cortex, which contributes to falls. Given that movement through the environment requires attending to relevant stimuli and inhibiting prepotent, yet potentially unsafe, responses, it is not surprising that brain regions associated with response inhibition and selective attention are related to falls risk.

Importantly, we found that activation in the left OFC/In was negatively associated with reduced physiological falls risk, whereas activation in the PCG/ACC was positively associated with reduced physiological falls risk. Increased activation in the frontal cortex during an executive task, such as the flanker, is associated with better task performance [31]. In contrast, increased activation of the anterior cingulate cortex in older adults is associated with reduced task performance [31]. In particular, increased anterior cingulate cortex activation is hypothesized to be an indicator of greater cognitive effort, such that the anterior cingulate cortex is less efficient at triggering the prefrontral system to engage cognitive control [45].

Our volumetric brain results also suggest that total white matter volume, rather than total grey matter volume, is associated with change in physiological falls risk. Previous research suggests that white matter declines at a faster rate than grey matter in otherwise healthy older adults [46]. Our results extends this finding by suggesting the loss of total white matter volume may be an early indicator of increased falls risk among community-dwelling older adults.

Of particular clinical relevance, the results of our study suggest that individuals at higher risk for future falls have greater potential for risk reduction than those at lower risk for falls. Specifically, our multiple regression model showed that baseline physiological falls risk was positively associated with change in physiological falls risk. Hence, our current study results concurs and extends that of a previous meta-analysis that concluded exercise-based falls prevention strategies are most effective among those at the greatest risk [47]. This suggests that one intervention strategy for falls prevention may be to target those who are at greatest risk for falls.

We note that of the independent variables included in our regression model, baseline activation of the left OFC/In was most associated with reduced physiological falls risk. Hence, while many falls interventions focus on balance training, our study suggests that future falls prevention strategies should potentially incorporate intervention components that induce neurocognitive plasticity (i.e., changes in brain function). Future work is needed to establish whether such interventions would be effective. Current evidence suggests that targeted aerobic exercise training has specific benefits on neurocognitive plasticity in brain regions that are responsible for selective attention and response inhibition [31]. Therefore, promoting plasticity in brain regions associated with these key executive functions may have a positive impact on falls prevention.

We acknowledge that our finding that a negative association between baseline total white matter volume and change in physiological falls risk is significantly associated with reduced falls risk contrasts previous cross-sectional studies on gray matter volume, balance, and mobility. Specifically, Rosano and colleagues [17, 18] found that reduced gait speed and impaired balance - key risk factors for falls -- were significantly correlated with reduced grey matter volume within sensorimotor and frontal parietal regions in the brain. However, we highlight that our study examined the independent contribution of baseline volumetric brain measures to change in falls risk (i.e., longitudinal study design versus cross-sectional design) and hence our conclusion that those at the greater risk for future falls (i.e., smaller baseline total white volume) have greater potential for falls risk reduction (i.e., greater change in PPA scores).

We recognize the limitations of our study. A key limitation is that we did not quantify white matter lesions within total white matter volume. We note that our study sample consisted exclusively of independent community-dwelling senior women who were without significant physical and cognitive impairments and without a significant history of falls. Thus, the results of our study may not generalize beyond this population of senior women and we may have underestimated the contribution of brain function to change in physiological falls risk. Future prospective studies are needed to test whether the present findings also apply to larger, more heterogeneous populations.

To conclude, the function of brain regions underlying response inhibition and selective attention was independently associated with reduced physiological falls risk. Hence, future falls prevention strategies should potentially incorporate intervention components, such as aerobic exercise training, that induce neurocognitive plasticity in the neural network that supports response inhibition and selective attention.

References

  1. Murray C, Lopez A: Global and regional descriptive epidemiology of disability: incidence, prevalence, health expectancies, and years lived with disability. The global burden of disease. Edited by: Murray C, Lopez A. 1996, Boston: The Harvard School of Public Health, 201-246.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Tinetti ME, Speechley M, Ginter SF: Risk factors for falls among elderly persons living in the community. The New England journal of medicine. 1988, 319: 1701-1707. 10.1056/NEJM198812293192604.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Lord S, Clark R, Webster I: Physiological factors associated with falls in an elderly population. Journal of American Geriatrics Society. 1991, 39: 1194-1200.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Lord S, Ward J, Williams P, Anstey K: Physiological factors associated with falls in older community-dwelling women. Journal of American Geriatrics Society. 1994, 42: 1110-1117.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Lundin-Olsson L, Nyberg L, Gustafson Y: "Stops walking when talking" as a predictor of falls in elderly people. Lancet. 1997, 349: 617-

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Anstey KJ, von Sanden C, Luszcz MA: An 8-year prospective study of the relationship between cognitive performance and falling in very old adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2006, 54: 1169-1176. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2006.00813.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Liu-Ambrose T, Ahamed Y, Graf P, Feldman F, Robinovitch SN: Older fallers with poor working memory overestimate their postural limits. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation. 2008, 89: 1335-1340. 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.11.052.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Liu-Ambrose TY, Ashe MC, Graf P, Beattie BL, Khan KM: Increased risk of falling in older community-dwelling women with mild cognitive impairment. Phys Ther. 2008, 88: 1482-1491. 10.2522/ptj.20080117.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Lord S, Fitzpatrick R: Choice stepping reaction time: A composite measure of fall risk in older people. Journal of gerontology. 2001, 10: M627-632.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Holtzer R, Friedman R, Lipton RB, Katz M, Xue X, Verghese J: The relationship between specific cognitive functions and falls in aging. Neuropsychology. 2007, 21: 540-548.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Persad CC, Giordani B, Chen HC, Ashton-Miller JA, Alexander NB, Wilson CS, Berent S, Guire K, Schultz AB: Neuropsychological predictors of complex obstacle avoidance in healthy older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1995, 50: P272-277.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Thal DR, Del Tredici K, Braak H: Neurodegeneration in normal brain aging and disease. Sci Aging Knowledge Environ. 2004, 2004: pe26-10.1126/sageke.2004.23.pe26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Briley DP, Wasay M, Sergent S, Thomas S: Cerebral white matter changes (leukoaraiosis), stroke, and gait disturbance. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1997, 45: 1434-1438.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Soumare A, Elbaz A, Zhu Y, Maillard P, Crivello F, Tavernier B, Dufouil C, Mazoyer B, Tzourio C: White matter lesions volume and motor performances in the elderly. Annals of neurology. 2009, 65: 706-715. 10.1002/ana.21674.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Masdeu JC, Wolfson L, Lantos G, Tobin JN, Grober E, Whipple R, Amerman P: Brain white-matter changes in the elderly prone to falling. Archives of neurology. 1989, 46: 1292-1296.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Baloh RW, Ying SH, Jacobson KM: A longitudinal study of gait and balance dysfunction in normal older people. Archives of neurology. 2003, 60: 835-839. 10.1001/archneur.60.6.835.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Rosano C, Aizenstein H, Brach J, Longenberger A, Studenski S, Newman AB: Special Article: Gait Measures Indicate Underlying Focal Gray Matter Atrophy in the Brain of Older Adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008, 63: 1380-1388.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Rosano C, Aizenstein HJ, Studenski S, Newman AB: A regions-of-interest volumetric analysis of mobility limitations in community-dwelling older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2007, 62: 1048-1055.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, Kim JS, Prakash R, McAuley E, Elavsky S, Marquez DX, Hu L, Kramer AF: Aerobic Exercise Training Increases Brain Volume in Aging Humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006, 61: 1166-1170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Liu-Ambrose TY, Nagamatsu LS, Handy TC, Leghari A: Does impaired cerebellar function contribute to risk of falls in seniors? A pilot study using functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008, 56: 2153-2155. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01984.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Graf P, Beattie BL, Ashe MC, Handy TC: Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Archives of internal medicine. 2010, 170: 170-178. 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.494.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975, 12: 189-198. 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Yesavage JA: Geriatric Depression Scale. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988, 24: 709-711.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Charlson ME, Pompei P, Ales KL, MacKenzie CR: A new method of classifying prognostic comorbidity in longitudinal studies: development and validation. Journal of chronic diseases. 1987, 40: 373-383. 10.1016/0021-9681(87)90171-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Smith SM, Zhang Y, Jenkinson M, Chen J, Matthews PM, Federico A, De Stefano N: Accurate, robust, and automated longitudinal and cross-sectional brain change analysis. NeuroImage. 2002, 17: 479-489. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE: Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage. 2004, 23: S208-S219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Smith SM: Fast robust automated brain extraction. Human brain mapping. 2002, 17: 143-155. 10.1002/hbm.10062.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Jenkinson M: Fast, automated, N-dimensional phase-unwrapping algorithm. Magn Reson Med. 2003, 49: 193-197. 10.1002/mrm.10354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Jenkinson M, Smith S: A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Medical image analysis. 2001, 5: 143-156. 10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Zhang Y, Brady M, Smith S: Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans on Medical Imaging. 2001, 20: 45-57. 10.1109/42.906424.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P, McAuley E, Cohen NJ, Webb A, Jerome GJ, Marquez DX, Elavsky S: Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004, 101: 3316-3321. 10.1073/pnas.0400266101.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Friston KJ, Ashburner J, Frith CD, Poline J-P, Heather JD: Spatial registration and normalization of images. Huamn Brain Mapp. 1995, 2: 165-189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Friston KJ, Frith CD, Turner R, Frackowiak RS: Characterizing evoked hemodynamics with fMRI. NeuroImage. 1995, 2: 157-165. 10.1006/nimg.1995.1018.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Talairach J, Tournoux P: Co-planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. 1988, New York: Thieme Medical Publishers

    Google Scholar 

  35. Rosano C, Aizenstein HJ, Wu M, Newman AB, Becker JT, Lopez OL, Kuller LH: Focal atrophy and cerebrovascular disease increase dementia risk among cognitively normal older adults. J Neuroimaging. 2007, 17: 148-155. 10.1111/j.1552-6569.2007.00093.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Rosano C, Brach J, Longstreth WT, Newman AB: Quantitative measures of gait characteristics indicate prevalence of underlying subclinical structural brain abnormalities in high-functioning older adults. Neuroepidemiology. 2006, 26: 52-60. 10.1159/000089240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Rosano C, Brach J, Studenski S, Longstreth WT, Newman AB: Gait variability is associated with subclinical brain vascular abnormalities in high-functioning older adults. Neuroepidemiology. 2007, 29: 193-200. 10.1159/000111582.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Rosano C, Newman AB, Katz R, Hirsch CH, Kuller LH: Association Between Lower Digit Symbol Substitution Test Score and Slower Gait and Greater Risk of Mortality and of Developing Incident Disability in Well-Functioning Older Adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2008, 56: 1618-1625. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01856.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Rosano C, Simonsick EM, Harris TB, Kritchevsky SB, Brach J, Visser M, Yaffe K, Newman AB: Association between physical and cognitive function in healthy elderly: the health, aging and body composition study. Neuroepidemiology. 2005, 24: 8-14. 10.1159/000081043.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM, Botvinick MM, Noll D, Cohen JD: Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science. 1998, 280: 747-749. 10.1126/science.280.5364.747.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Casey BJ, Trainor RJ, Orendi JL, Schubert AB, Nystrom LE, Giedd JN, Castellanos FX, Haxby JV, Noll DC, Cohen JD: A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation during performance of a Go-No-Go task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1997, 9: 835-847. 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.6.835.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Haupt S, Axmacher N, Cohen MX, Elger CE, Fell J: Activation of the caudal anterior cingulate cortex due to task-related interference in an auditory stroop. Human Brain Mapping. 2009, 30: 3043-3056. 10.1002/hbm.20731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Boehler CN, Appelbaum LG, Krebs RM, Hopf JM, Woldorff MG: Pinning down response inhibition in the brain - Conjunction analyses of the Stop-signal task. NeuroImage. 2010, 52: 1621-1632. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.276.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Anstey KJ, Wood J, Kerr G, Caldwell H, Lord SR: Different cognitive profiles for single compared with recurrent fallers without dementia. Neuropsychology. 2009, 23: 500-508.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Botvinick MM: Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconsiling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioural Neuroscience. 2007, 7: 356-366. 10.3758/CABN.7.4.356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Resnick SM, Pham DL, Kraut MA, Zonderman AB, Davatzikos C: Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies of older adults: A shrinking brain. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003, 23: 3295-3301.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Robertson MC, Campbell AJ, Gardner MM, Devlin N: Preventing Injuries in Older People by Preventing Falls: A Meta-Analysis of Individual-Level Data. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002, 50: 905-911. 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50218.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Kramer AF, Hahn S, Gopher D: Task coordination and aging: explorations of executive control processes in the task switching paradigm. Acta Psychol (Amst). 1999, 101: 339-378. 10.1016/S0001-6918(99)00011-6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements and Funding

The authors would like to thank the Vancouver South Slope YMCA management and members who enthusiastically supported the study by allowing access to participants for the training intervention. We thank the instructors for their commitment to the participants' health and safety. TLA is a Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research (MSFHR) Scholar. LSN is a MSFHR Senior Graduate trainee and a NSERC Doctoral trainee.

This work was supported by the Vancouver Foundation (BCM06-0035), the Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research Establishment Grant (CI-SCH-063(05-1)CLIN), and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MOB-93373) to TLA.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Teresa Liu-Ambrose.

Additional information

Declaration of Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

TLA conceived and designed the study, acquired data, and analyzed and interpreted the data. LSN and CLH acquired data and participated in the statistical analysis. TLA, LSN, CLH, and TCH drafted and revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nagamatsu, L.S., Hsu, C.L., Handy, T.C. et al. Functional neural correlates of reduced physiological falls risk. Behav Brain Funct 7, 37 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-7-37

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-7-37

Keywords