Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Exercise is medicine, for the body and the brain
  1. Lindsay S Nagamatsu1,
  2. Leon Flicker2,
  3. Arthur F Kramer3,
  4. Michelle W Voss4,
  5. Kirk I Erickson5,
  6. Chun Liang Hsu6,
  7. Teresa Liu-Ambrose6,7,8
  1. 1 Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
  2. 2 Centre for Medical Research, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
  3. 3 Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, Illinois, USA
  4. 4 Department of Psychology, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa, USA
  5. 5 Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, USA
  6. 6 Department of Physical Therapy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
  7. 7 Brain Research Centre, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
  8. 8 Centre for Hip Health and Mobility, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
  1. Correspondence to Dr Teresa Liu-Ambrose, Department of Physical Therapy, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z3; tlambrose{at}exchange.ubc.ca

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Cognitive decline is one of the most pressing healthcare issues of the 21st century. Worldwide, one new case of major cognitive decline (ie, dementia) is detected every 4 s.1 Given that no effective pharmacological treatment to alter the progress of cognitive decline exists, there is much interest in lifestyle approaches for preventing or treating dementia. Ideally, such strategies should be cost-efficient and widely accessible at a societal level to have the largest benefit for older adults with varying income and functional status levels.

One attractive solution that aligns with the above criteria is exercise. However, despite a large and consistent pool of evidence generated over the past five decades linking exercise to improved cognitive functions in older adults,2 there is a reluctance among academics, healthcare practitioners and the public alike to embrace exercise as a prevention and treatment strategy for cognitive decline. For example, the National Institutes of Health (NIH) consensus statement from 20103 concedes that there appears to be preliminary data to support the efficacy of exercise in improving cognitive function. However, they caution that there is currently no strong evidence to suggest that modifiable lifestyle factors can alter the trajectory of cognitive decline. Adding fuel to the fire are publications such as a 2013 systematic review of randomised controlled trials (RCTs) (prior to 31 October 2011) reporting ‘weak’ evidence for the effects of exercise on cognition.4 We must highlight that the search strategy used in that systematic review failed to capture many pertinent papers providing evidence from RCTs that exercise promotes cognitive and brain plasticity not only in healthy older adults but also in those with cognitive impairment. Furthermore, there are a number of animal studies that provide insight into the molecular and cellular mechanisms by which exercise promotes neuroplasticity.5

In a previous commentary,6 we outlined a selection of studies that provided compelling evidence that exercise should undoubtedly be considered as a promising treatment strategy for cognitive decline. Our present commentary aims to provide a brief update, as well as to discuss factors that have continued to hinder the adoption of exercise as a legitimate medical strategy for the prevention of cognitive decline and dementia.

Since 2010, we have additional evidence from RCTs that exercise, both moderate-to-vigorous intensity aerobic and resistance training, promotes cognitive and brain plasticity and have gained further insight into underlying mechanisms. Notably, the behavioural and mechanistic outcomes converge and support a cohesive picture. In 2011, Erickson et al 7 demonstrated that aerobic exercise resulted in increased hippocampal volume in healthy community-dwelling older adults. Furthermore, changes in hippocampal volume in the aerobic exercise group were significantly associated with increased levels of serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor and spatial memory performance. In the same RCT, Voss et al 8 demonstrated that aerobic exercise also improved the functional connectivity or temporal coherence of brain regions that are functionally related in a network known to decouple with ageing. For resistance training, Liu-Ambrose et al 9 demonstrated increased functional plasticity after 12 months of training with corresponding improvement in selective attention and conflict resolution in healthy older women. Critically, the benefits of exercise extend to those with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia. Among older women with MCI, Nagamatsu et al 10 ,11 demonstrated that 6 months of resistance training led to improved executive functions, spatial memory and associative memory with concurring functional plasticity. In the same study,11 the authors also found that aerobic training improved verbal memory and learning. These findings indeed support the findings from a recent systematic review on exercise and cognition in those with dementia,12 although to our knowledge no such systematic reviews exist for those with MCI.

Despite these recent positive findings, scepticism remains. Enthusiasm for increased exercise as a prevention strategy is often tempered by the fact that no RCT until now has demonstrated that exercise can actually reduce the incidence of MCI or dementia. Is the absence of such a definitive trial reason enough to disregard the evidence until now? We contend that the answer to this question is a resounding ‘no’ when the following points are considered:

  • The number of individuals with dementia will exponentially increase in the next 20 years.

  • There is now a large and consistent pool of animal and human data demonstrating the cognitive benefit of exercise. Importantly, recent randomised studies show a convergence among behavioural, neuroimaging, and serum biomarker outcomes. Furthermore, large cohort studies have demonstrated significant associations between exercise and reduced risk of cognitive impairment and dementia.13–16

  • Exercise has a multitude of established health benefits with minimal side effects and is cost-effective.17 Even in older adults, exercise increases the chance of survival and healthy ageing.18

  • Exercise significantly reduces the key vascular risk factors19 (eg, hypertension, diabetes type II, hypercholesterolaemia, etc) for Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia—the two most common types of dementia.20

In fact, exercise should be promoted as an essential component of healthy ageing given that reducing physical inactivity by 25% could prevent as many as one million cases of dementia worldwide.21

Providing healthcare practitioners and seniors with specific and individualised recommendations regarding the type, intensity, and frequency of exercise for optimal benefit is critical to successfully advocate exercise as a treatment for cognitive decline. However, we are limited by the fact that dose and type of exercise have been utilised in different studies with varying success. For example, Baker et al 22 found that older women with MCI who engaged in aerobic exercise four times per week exhibited improved cognitive function, whereas in a similar population, aerobic exercise twice per week did not yield the same benefits.10 Further, confusion centres around the type of exercise (eg, aerobic vs resistance training)10 and sex differences.22 Our ability to translate the evidence that has demonstrated that exercise can impact cognitive function also depends on an in-depth understanding of the science behind the phenomenon, especially since the public may not appreciate that exercise can alter the structure and function of the brain directly. Future evidence linking exercise and cognition will surely boost the credibility of exercise as a treatment strategy, much in the same way that pharmacological alternatives are currently universally accepted. Therefore, the priority for future research is to elucidate the type or combination of exercise that yields the greatest cognitive benefits for each individual based on factors such as sex, age, and current cognitive status, as well as underlying mechanisms that support such changes.

In this commentary, we have presented an updated account of the accumulating behavioural, neuroimaging and biomarker evidence linking exercise and cognition. We acknowledge that we are not yet able to provide clinicians and older adults with precise ‘prescriptions’ of exercise that would maximise the cognitive benefits for different degrees and causes of cognitive impairment; nor do we completely understand the underlying molecular and cellular mechanisms. These knowledge gaps will certainly be addressed in future research. Neverthless, current evidence strongly suggests that exercise does have benefits for cognition—and critically, the magnitude of the benefit can exceed that of pharmaceutical approaches.23 Of relevance, it is estimated that if the onset and progression of dementia could be delayed by a mere 1 year, there would be nearly 9.2 million fewer cases in 2050.24 Given that exercise can be promoted at a population level with relatively low risks and costs, we highly recommend that older adults should be encouraged to engage in regular exercise to boost cognitive function as further research is concurrently conducted, rather than wait until all the intricacies of the relationship between exercise and cognition have been resolved.

During a recent symposium that brought together world experts in this expanding field (International Symposium on Exercise to Promote Cognitive Health and Functional Independence, May 2010), one of the most prominent themes was how to translate our knowledge about the benefits of exercise to healthcare providers and the general public, and how to engage older adults in exercise with high adherence. We recognise that a large proportion of the population is sedentary and that successfully changing behaviour to a more active lifestyle is challenging. To effectively advocate exercise, we emphasise the need for a cross-disciplinary collaborative effort among researchers and healthcare workers to develop consistent standards and a united message of the multiple benefits of exercise. In light of the recent support for exercise in preventing cognitive decline, we stress that endorsing exercise as an effective strategy for improving health and well-being among older adults should be indisputable. Given that it may potentially have a high impact for mitigating multiple health concerns, including dementia, exercise should therefore be considered as a leading treatment strategy to delay or prevent dementia.

References

View Abstract

Footnotes

  • Contributors LSN, LF, AFK, MWV, KIE, CLH and TLA contributed to the conception, writing, and critical review of this manuscript.

  • Competing interests TL-A is a Canada Research Chair in Physical Activity, Mobility, and Cognitive Neuroscience. LSN is a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Doctoral trainee. KIE is supported by NIH grant ROI DK095172. CLH is an Alzheimer's Society Research Program Doctoral trainee.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.